Read online free
  • Home
  • Romance & Love
  • Fantasy
  • Science Fiction
  • Mystery & Detective
  • Thrillers & Crime
  • Actions & Adventure
  • History & Fiction
  • Horror
  • Western
  • Humor

    Not Born Yesterday

    Prev Next


      truth. In the face of such motivations, open vigilance mecha-

      nisms come to be used, perversely, to identify not the most

      plausible but the most implausible views.

      From the most intuitive to the most preposterous, if we want

      to understand why some mistaken views catch on, we must un-

      derstand how open vigilance works.

      Uptake

      At the end of the book, you should have a grasp on how you de-

      cide what to believe and who to trust. You should know more

      about how miserably unsuccessful most attempts at mass persua-

      sion are, from the most banal— advertising, proselytizing—to

      the most extreme— brainwashing, subliminal influence. You

      should have some clues about why (some) mistaken ideas man-

      in t r o d u c t i o n xix

      age to spread, while (some) valuable insights prove so difficult

      to diffuse. You should understand why I once gave a fake doctor

      twenty euros.

      I do hope you come to accept the core of the book’s argument.

      But, please, don’t just take my word for it. I’d hate to be proven

      wrong by my own readers.

      1

      THE CASE FOR GULLIBILITY

      for millennia, people have accepted many bizarre beliefs

      and have been persuaded to engage in irrational be hav iors (or

      so it appears). These beliefs and be hav iors gave credence to the

      idea that the masses are gullible. In real ity I believe the story is

      more complicated (or even completely diff er ent, as we’ll see in

      the following chapters). But I must start by laying out the case

      for gullibility.

      In 425 BCE, Athens had been locked for years in a mutually

      destructive war with Sparta. At the Battle of Pylos, the Athenian

      naval and ground forces managed to trap Spartan troops on the

      island of Sphacteria. Seeing that a significant number of their elite

      were among the captives, the Spartan leaders sued for peace, of-

      fering advantageous terms to Athens. The Athenians declined

      the offer. The war went on, Sparta regained the edge, and when

      a (temporary) peace treaty was signed, in 421 BCE, the terms

      were much less favorable to Athens. This blunder was only one

      of a series of terrible Athenian decisions. Some were morally

      repellent— kil ing all the citizens of a conquered city— others

      were strategically disastrous— launching a doomed expedition

      to Sicily. In the end, Athens lost the war and would never regain

      its former power.

      1

      2 ch ap t er 1

      In 1212, a “multitude of paupers” in France and Germany took

      the cross to fight the infidels and reclaim Jerusalem for the Catho-

      lic Church.1 As many of these paupers were very young, this

      movement was dubbed the Children’s Crusade. The youth made

      it to Saint- Denis, prayed in the cathedral, met the French king,

      hoped for a miracle. No miracle happened. What can be expected

      of an army of untrained, unfunded, disor ga nized preteens? Not

      much, which is what they achieved: none reached Jerusalem, and

      many died along the way.

      In the mid- eighteenth century the Xhosa, a pastoralist people

      of South Africa, were suffering under the newly imposed British

      rule. Some of the Xhosa believed kil ing all their cattle and burn-

      ing their crops would raise a ghost army that would fend off the

      British. They sacrificed thousands of heads of cattle and set

      fire to their fields. No ghost army arose. The British stayed. The

      Xhosa died.

      On December 4, 2016, Edgar Maddison Welch entered the

      Comet Ping Pong pizzeria in Washington, DC, carry ing an as-

      sault rifle, a revolver, and a shotgun. He wasn’t there to rob the

      restaurant. Instead, he wanted to make sure that no children were

      being held hostage in the basement. There had been rumors that

      the Clintons— the former U.S. president and his wife, then cam-

      paigning for the presidency— were running a sex trafficking

      ring, and that Comet Ping Pong was one of their lairs. Welch was

      arrested and is now serving a prison sentence.

      Blind Trust

      Scholars, feeling superior to the masses, have often explained

      these questionable decisions and weird beliefs by a human dis-

      position to be overly trusting, a disposition that would make the

      masses instinctively defer to charismatic leaders regardless of

      t he c a se f o r g ul l ib il i t y 3

      their competence or motivations, believe what ever they hear or

      read irrespective of its plausibility, and follow the crowd even

      when doing so leads to disaster. This explanation— the masses

      are credulous— has proven very influential throughout history

      even if, as will soon become clear, it is misguided.

      Why did the Athenians lose the war against Sparta? Starting

      with Thucydides, chronicler of the Peloponnesian War, many

      commentators have blamed the influence of demagogues such

      as Cleon, a parvenu “very power ful with the multitude,” who was

      deemed responsible for some of the war’s worst blunders.2 A

      generation later, Plato extended Thucydides’s argument into

      a general indictment of democracy. For Plato, the rule of the

      many unavoidably gives rise to leaders who, “having a mob

      entirely at [their] disposal,” turn into tyrants.3

      Why would a bunch of youngsters abandon their homes in the

      vain hope of invading a faraway land? They were responding to

      the calls for a new crusade launched by Pope Innocent III, their

      supposed credulity inspiring the legend of the Pied Piper of

      Hamelin, whose magic flute grants him absolute power over all

      the children who hear it.4 People’s crusades also help explain the

      accusations that emerged in the Enlightenment, by the likes of

      the Baron d’Holbach, who chastised the Christian Church for

      “deliver[ing] mankind into [the] hands of [despots and tyrants]

      as a herd of slaves, of whom they may dispose at their

      plea sure.”5

      Why did the Xhosa kill their cattle? A century earlier, the Mar-

      quis de Condorcet, a central figure of the French Enlighten-

      ment, suggested that members of small- scale socie ties suffered

      from the “credulity of the first dupes,” putting too much faith in

      “charlatans and sorcerers.”6 The Xhosa seem to fit this picture.

      They were taken in by Nongqawuse, a young prophetess who

      had had visions of the dead rising to fight the British, and of a

      4 ch ap t er 1

      new world in which “nobody would ever lead a troubled life.

      People would get what ever they wanted. Every thing would be

      available in abundance.”7 Who would say no to that? Apparently

      not the Xhosa.

      Why did Edgar Maddison Welch risk jail to deliver non ex-

      is tent children from the non ex is tent basement of a harmless

      pizzeria? He had been listening to Alex Jones, the charismatic

      radio host who specializes in the craziest conspiracy theories,

      from the great Satanist takeover of Amer ica to government-

      sponsored calamities.8 For a time, Jones took up the idea that

      the Clintons and their aides led an organ ization trafficki
    ng

      children for sex. As a Washington Post reporter put it, Jones and

      his ilk can peddle their wild theories because “gullibility helps

      create a market for it.”9

      All of these observers agree that people are often credulous,

      easily accept unsubstantiated arguments, and are routinely talked

      into stupid and costly be hav iors. Indeed, it is difficult to find an

      idea that so well unites radically diff er ent thinkers. Preachers lam-

      baste the “credulous multitude” who believe in gods other than

      the preachers’ own.10 Atheists point out “the almost superhuman

      gullibility” of those who follow religious preachers, what ever

      their god might be.11 Conspiracy theorists feel superior to the

      “mind controlled sheeple” who accept the official news.12 De-

      bunkers think conspiracy theorists “super gul ible” for believing

      the tall tales peddled by angry entertainers.13 Conservative writ-

      ers accuse the masses of criminal credulity when they revolt,

      prodded by shameless demagogues and driven mad by conta-

      gious emotions. Old- school leftists explain the passivity of the

      masses by their ac cep tance of the dominant ideology: “The in-

      dividual lives his repression ‘freely’ as his own life: he desires

      what he is supposed to desire,” instead of acting on “his original

      instinctual needs.”14

      t he c a se f o r g ul l ib il i t y 5

      For most of history, the concept of widespread credulity has

      been fundamental to our understanding of society. The assump-

      tion that people are easily taken in by demagogues runs across

      Western thought, from ancient Greece to the Enlightenment,

      creating “po liti cal philosophy’s central reason for skepticism

      about democracy.”15 Con temporary commenters still deplore

      how easily politicians sway voters by “pander[ing] to their gull-

      ibility.”16 But the ease with which people can be influenced has

      never been so (apparently) well illustrated as through a number

      of famous experiments conducted by social psychologists since

      the 1950s.

      Psychologists of Gullibility

      First came Solomon Asch. In his most famous experiment he

      asked people to answer a simple question: Which of three lines

      (depicted in figure 1) is as long as the first line?17 The three

      lines were clearly of diff er ent lengths, and one of them was an

      obvious match for the first. Yet participants made a mistake

      more than 30 percent of the time. Why would people provide

      such blatantly wrong answers? Before each participant was

      asked for their opinion, several participants had already re-

      plied. Unbeknownst to the actual participant, these other par-

      ticipants were confederates, planted by the experimenter. On

      some trials, all the confederates agreed on one of the wrong

      answers. These confederates held no power over the partici-

      pants, who did not even know them, and they were providing

      plainly wrong answers. Stil , more than 60 percent of partici-

      pants chose at least once to fol ow the group’s lead. A textbook

      written by Serge Moscovici, an influential social psychologist,

      describes these results as “one of the most dramatic illustra-

      tions of conformity, of blindly going along with the group, even

      6 ch ap t er 1

      A

      B

      C

      Figure 1. The lines in the Asch conformity experiments. Source: Wikipedia.

      when the individual realizes that by doing so he turns his back

      on real ity and truth.”18

      After Solomon Asch came Stanley Milgram. Milgram’s first

      famous study was, like Asch’s experiments, a study of conformity.

      He asked some of his students to stand on a sidewalk, looking

      at a building’s win dow, and counted how many of the people

      passing by would imitate them.19 When enough students were

      looking in the same direction— the critical group size seemed

      to be about five— nearly all those who passed by followed the

      students in looking at the building. It was as if people could not

      help but follow the crowd.

      But Milgram is best known for a later, much more provoca-

      tive experiment.20 In this study, participants were asked to take

      t he c a se f o r g ul l ib il i t y 7

      part in research bearing ostensibly on learning. In the lab, they

      were introduced to another participant— who, once again, was

      actually a confederate. The experimenter pretended to randomly

      pick one of the two— always the confederate—to be the learner.

      Participants were then told the study tested whether someone

      who was motivated to avoid electric shocks would learn better.

      The learner had to memorize a list of words; when he made a

      mistake, the participant would be asked to administer an elec-

      tric shock.

      The participants sat in front of a big machine with a series of

      switches corresponding to electric shocks of increasingly high

      voltage. The confederate was led slightly away, to an experimen-

      tal booth, but the participants could still hear him through a

      microphone. At first, the confederate did a good enough job

      memorizing the words, but as the task grew more difficult, he

      started making mistakes. The experimenter prompted the par-

      ticipants to shock the confederate, and all of them did. This was

      hardly surprising, as the first switches were marked as deliver-

      ing only a “slight shock.” As the confederate kept making

      mistakes, the experimenter urged the participants to increase the

      voltage. The switches went from “slight shock,” to “moderate

      shock,” then “strong shock,” and “very strong shock,” yet all the

      participants kept flipping the switches. It was only on the last

      switch of the “intense shock” series—300 volts— that a few par-

      ticipants refused to proceed. All the while, the confederate ex-

      pressed his discomfort. At some point, he started howling in

      pain, begging the participants to stop: “Let me out of here! You

      can’t hold me here! Get me out of here!”21 He even complained

      of heart prob lems. Yet the vast majority of participants kept

      going.

      When the “extreme intensity shock” series began, a few more

      participants stopped. One participant refused to go on when the

      8 ch ap t er 1

      switches indicated “danger: severe shock.” At this stage, the con-

      federate had simply stopped screaming and was begging to be

      freed. He then became completely unresponsive. But that didn’t

      stop two- thirds of the participants from flipping the last two

      switches, 435 volts and 450 volts, marked with an ominous

      “XXX.” Milgram had gotten a substantial majority of these or-

      dinary American citizens to deliver (what they thought to be)

      potentially lethal electric shocks to a fellow citizen who (they

      thought) was writhing in pain and begging for mercy.

      When learning of these results, and of a litany of historical

      cases seemingly attesting to similar phenomena, it is hard not to

      agree with the sweeping indictment leveled by po liti cal phi los-

      o pher Jason
    Brennan: “ Human beings are wired not to seek truth

      and justice but to seek consensus. They are shackled by social

      pressure. They are overly deferential to authority. They cower

      before uniform opinion. They are swayed not so much by rea-

      son but by a desire to belong, by emotional appeal, and by sex

      appeal.”22 Psychologist Daniel Gilbert and his colleagues con-

      cur: “That human beings are, in fact, more gullible than they are

      suspicious should prob ably ‘be counted among the first and most

      common notions that are innate in us.’ ”23

      If you believe that humans are by nature credulous, the natu-

      ral question to ask is: Why? Already in 500 BCE Heraclitus, one

      of the first recorded Greek phi los o phers, was wondering:

      What use are the people’s wits

      who let themselves be led

      by speechmakers, in crowds,

      without considering

      how many fools and thieves

      they are among, and how few

      choose the good?24

      t he c a se f o r g ul l ib il i t y 9

      Heraclitus was echoed twenty- five hundred years later in a less

      poetic but more concise manner by this headline from the BBC:

      “Why are people so incredibly gullible?”25

      Adaptive Credulity

      If social psychologists seem to have been bent on demonstrat-

      ing human credulity, anthropologists have, for the most part,

      taken it for granted.26 Many have seen the per sis tence of tradi-

      tional beliefs and be hav iors as unproblematic: children simply

      imbibe the culture that surrounds them, thereby ensuring its

      continuity. Logically, anthropologists have devoted little at-

      tention to children, who are supposed to be mere receptacles for

      the knowledge and skil s of the previous generation.27 Critical

      anthropologists have described the assumption that people

      absorb what ever culture surrounds them as the theory of “ex-

      haustive cultural transmission,”28 or, more pejoratively, as the

      “ ‘fax model’ of internalization.”29

      For all its simplicity, this model of cultural transmission helps

      us understand why people would be credulous: so they learn the

      knowledge and skil s acquired by generations of their ancestors.

      Biologist Richard Dawkins thus explains the “programmed-in

      gullibility of a child” by its “useful[ness] for learning language

      and traditional wisdom.”30

      While it is easy to think of “traditional wisdom” one would

     

    Prev Next
Read online free - Copyright 2016 - 2025